Mindful Eating Training Can Help Reduce Impulsive Eating Habits

Do you find yourself eating even when you are not hungry? Do you have trouble resisting impulsive eating throughout the day? Overweight and obese individuals tend to be more impulsive and sensitive to food rewards compared to normal weight individuals. Obesity is a growing health concern, with over 2/3 of the U.S. population being overweight or obese (CDC, 2015). A recent study published in the journal Health Psychology investigated the effects of a mindful-eating training on impulsive food choice in both adolescents and adults. Mindful eating is an approach to eating where you learn to pay attention to your thoughts, feelings and sensations both during and after eating.

In this study, 348 adults and adolescents participated in two study sessions. The first session was to measure the baseline discounting rate. The baseline rate was compared to the discounting rate after the intervention in order to determine the amount of change. During the first session, participants completed a delayed discounting task, a task that measures impulsivity by comparing the preference for smaller, more immediate rewards over larger, delayed rewards. For the second session, participants were randomized to a mindful eating group, DVD control, or a standard control group. The mindful-eating group completed a 50-min workshop on mindful eating where they applied their newly learned skills while eating four types of foods. The DVD control group watched a 50-min DVD about nutrition and the food pyramid. The standard control group did not participate in any training or standardized activity.

Both adolescents and adults who participated in the mindful-eating session experienced reduced impulsivity towards food choices from the baseline session. In addition, consistent with prior research individuals, both adolescents and adults, with higher percent body fat had higher food impulsivity than healthy weight individuals.

Strengths of this study include the randomized control design. This means that there was a control group and an intervention group. Also, participants were randomly assigned to their group, which ensures that the groups are equal on everything except the intervention. This design allows for more conclusive evidence about the effect of mindful eating training on impulsive eating compared to other designs. Findings from this study suggest that mindful eating interventions can decrease impulsive food choices, which could lead to weight loss. Mindful-eating training could also be a potential prevention strategy for impulsive eating, thus preventing obesity. Future research should investigate the long-term effects of mindful-eating interventions since this study only looked at short-term effects.

Hendrickson, K. L., & Rasmussen, E. B. (2017). Mindful eating reduces impulsive food choice in adolescents and adults. Health Psychology36(3), 226.

Posted in Uncategorized | Leave a comment

What Explains How People Feel During and After Exercise?

We all know that exercising is good for us. Yet, few of us engage in regular exercise. One reason individuals may choose to exercise or to not to exercise is that an individual may feel good or bad during exercise. For example, the better someone feels during exercise, the more likely they are to exercise regularly.

In a recently published study from our lab we identified factors that help explain how people feel during and after exercise. Specifically, we had healthy young adults complete measures of anticipatory affect factors (affective attitudes, implicit associations, and affective associations), anticipated affect factors (anticipated regret, anticipated pride), and cognitive factors (self-efficacy, intentions). Anticipatory affect is the experience of pleasant or unpleasant feelings while thinking about or anticipating exercise. Anticipated affect is the expectation of how one will feel after completing an exercise session, such as anticipated pride or anticipated regret. Cognitive factors include self-efficacy (i.e., the belief that one is able to perform a specific behavior) and behavioral intentions (i.e., a belief about one’s readiness to perform a behavior). After completing measures, participants ran or walked for 20-minutes on a treadmill and answered questions on how they were feeling during and after the exercise.

We found that both regret and pride (anticipated affect) and anticipatory affect variables accounted for unique aspects of how people feel during and after exercise. Only anticipatory affect uniquely accounted for affective response immediately post-exercise. There were no associations between the affective and cognitive factors and affective response 5-10 minutes post-exercise. We also found that the associations between exercise self-efficacy and how people feel during exercise was due to the overlap that self-efficacy has with affective factors.

Overall, we found unique associations between affective, but not cognitive, factors and affective response to exercise. Findings from this study highlight the importance of considering multiple factors simultaneously to understand unique predictors to affective response. Future research should identify the extent to which these various factors overlap and the extent to which they are distinct.  Findings also suggest that it is possible that intervening on affective factors may result in improved affective response to exercise, which could increase exercise behavior. Future research should examine this possibility.

Sala, M., Baldwin, A. S., & Williams, D. M. (2016). Affective and cognitive predictors of affective response to exercise: Examining unique and overlapping variance. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 27, 1-8.

Posted in Uncategorized | Leave a comment

Do Family Members of Melanoma Patients Use Sun Protection Behaviors?

The American Cancer Society estimates that 10,000 people a year will die from melanoma, a deadly form of skin cancer primarily caused by sun exposure. First-degree relatives of melanoma patients, such as parents and children, are at particularly high risk for developing melanoma themselves. One would imagine that with higher risk of developing skin cancer, first-degree relatives of melanoma patients would be more likely to engage in sun protective behaviors than others. However, is this actually the case?

A study recently published in Health Psychology examined the use of four sun-protective behaviors: sunscreen usage, shade-seeking, hat wearing, and protective clothing use among first-degree relatives of melanoma patients. The researchers assessed these behaviors twice a day in 59 participants over 14-days using an automated phone call system. Data collected from daily surveys over multiple days allows researchers to examine and understand behavior in real-time, and allows for exploring changes in behavior within individual participants that increases confidence in the reliability and robustness of the findings.

Among the four behaviors examined, sunscreen use was most frequent, followed by shade seeking, wearing hats, and wearing protective clothing. However, the majority of participants reported inconsistent use of these behaviors meaning most participants used each sun protection behavior less than 80% of the time. In addition, the study found that the factors leading to sun protective behaviors were different for different participants. This means that different environmental factors, such as time of day and level of sun exposure, influenced individual’s use of sun protection behaviors. This finding is important because it indicates that sun protection interventions may need to target people’s own reasons for using sun protection behavior.

Taken together, sun protection behaviors are inconsistently used in individuals at increased risk of developing skin cancer. Moreover, factors leading to sun protective behaviors vary for different individuals. Future research should examine associations between multiple sun protection behaviors, and develop personally tailored interventions to increase individuals’ use of sun protection behaviors.

Reference: Hay, J.L., Shuk, E., Schofield, E., Loeb, R., Holland, S., Burkhalter, J., & Li, Y. (2017). Real-time sun protection decisions in first-degree relatives of melanoma patients. Health Psychology, 36, 907-915.

Posted in Uncategorized | Leave a comment

An App to Help Parents Make a Decision about the HPV Vaccine

Although the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) has approved a vaccine that protects against the human papillomavirus (HPV) – a sexually transmitted virus that can cause several cancer types, including cervical cancer, current vaccination rates for the 3-dose series among US adolescents are low (39.7% for girls, 21.6% for boys).

In a recent paper our group published in the journal Patient Education and Counseling, we describe a new mobile app-based intervention we developed to address the problem of low HPV vaccination rates. The app is based on self-persuasion, the process of generating one’s own arguments for engaging in a specific behavior. Self-persuasion may be a particularly useful strategy for increasing adolescent HPV vaccination rates because many parents of adolescents are undecided or hesitant about the vaccine, and using parents’ own arguments for the vaccine may help them decide.

In this paper, we report on the development of the mobile app to promote parental self-persuasion for adolescent HPV vaccination and the evaluation of whether the app is easy and practical for parents to use. As part of the research study, parents viewed an informational video about HPV and the vaccine, answered a number of questions about different topics related to the vaccine, and then verbalized in their own words why it is important for their adolescent to get the vaccine.

We found that parents rated the app as easy to use and helpful for thinking about the HPV vaccine. In addition, although most of the parents were undecided about the vaccine when they began the study, 82% of parents reported deciding to get their adolescent vaccinated after using the app.

The self-persuasion app we developed is an excellent example of translating an established and effective behavior change approach – self-persuasion – into an innovative and effective way to impact population health. By developing the intervention within a mobile app, there is strong potential to make the intervention widely available to parents of adolescents who are making decisions about the HPV vaccine.

Reference. Baldwin, A.S., Denman, D.C., Sala, M., Marks, E.G., Shay, L.A., Fuller, S., Persaud, D., Lee, S.C., Skinner, C.S., Wiebe, D.J., & Tiro, J.T. (2017). Translating self-persuasion into an HPV vaccine promotion intervention for safety-net patients. Patient Education and Counseling, 100, 736-741.

Posted in Uncategorized | Leave a comment

Daily Self-Weighing: The Secret to Successful Weight Loss Maintenance

Is it useful to step on the scale every day? Findings from a recent study suggest that daily self-weighing might be the key to successful weight loss maintenance.

In a recently published study in Annals of Behavioral Medicine  researchers examined why a successful weight loss maintenance intervention –“Keep it Off – Guided Intervention”– has been successful. Keep it Off is an intervention designed to help people who have already lost weight to maintain their weight loss. Participants in the Keep it Off Guided intervention received 16 coaching sessions over two years targeting several factors that have been suggested to influence weight-loss maintenance. This intervention was compared to a control condition where participants received two phone coaching sessions and a course book with the topics key to weight loss-maintenance. This study focused on identifying mediators of the intervention – variables that account for the success of the intervention. Possible mediators included: starting self-weighting, stopping self-weighting, minutes of physical activity, calorie consumption, using weight-control strategies, amount of meals eaten at fast food venues, body image, and TV-related eating.

Of note, an important strength of this study was that participants were randomly assigned to the control or intervention groups, and the potential mediators for maintained weight loss were measured every 6 months over two years. These study design strengths increase confidence in the findings.

The researchers found that starting daily self-weighing and not stopping daily self-weighing were the only significant mediators of the intervention. Given the findings from this study and other studies which have documented the benefits of daily self-weighing, individuals who have lost weight and are seeking to maintain it should consider starting and not stopping self-weighing. In addition, several potential explanations for weight loss maintenance emerged, including eating away from home, dietary intake, and body satisfaction. These variables were predictive of weight changes but did not mediate the effect of the intervention. The lack of intervention effect on these factors that were associated with weight change could be due to a lack of emphasis on these behaviors in the intervention. Focusing on targeting these factors could strengthen future weight loss maintenance interventions, and these pathways should be investigated in future weight loss maintenance interventions.

Crain, A.L., Sherwood, N.E., Martinson, B.C., & Jeffery, R.W. (2017). Mediators of weight loss maintenance in the Keep It Off Trial. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 1-10.

Posted in Uncategorized | Leave a comment

Having Trouble Staying Active? Try Increasing Your Psychological Well-Being First

We know exercise is good for our health yet most of us struggle to exercise regularly, especially as we get older. A recent paper published in the Annals of Behavioral Medicine suggests that increasing your psychological well-being may lead to increases in physical activity, even later in adulthood. Psychological well-being includes positive feelings, distress, life satisfaction, optimism, and purpose in life.

In this longitudinal study, researchers investigated if higher psychological well-being increases the likelihood of meeting recommended physical activity levels, defined as 150 minutes of moderate or 75 minutes of vigorous-intensity exercise a week by the Center for Disease Control and Prevention (2013). Almost 10,000 older adults (>50 years old) were followed and surveyed every 2 years for 11 years. Longitudinal studies gather data from the same individuals over a period of time and are strong designs because it allows researchers to follow change over time. Researchers measured psychological well-being, which included questions about distress, control, autonomy, pleasure and self-realization. Participants also completed measures of frequency and intensity of their physical activity.

The researchers found that higher psychological well-being was with higher physical activity over the period of 11 years. They also found that among those who were initially inactive, those with higher psychological well-being were more likely to start regular physical activity. In addition, researchers statistically controlled for the influence of other variables, such as health status and psychological distress, to rule out the possibility that it was these other variables influencing the results.

The findings suggest that higher levels of psychological well-being predict future increases in physical activity. Therefore, psychological well-being may be a potential target for intervention. Evidence from various studies suggests that some ways to increase psychological well-being include writing exercises about positive life events and best possible future outcomes, as well as exercises to enhance positive emotions and personal values.

Kim, E. S., Kubzansky, L. D., Soo, J., & Boehm, J. K. (2017). Maintaining healthy behavior: A prospective study of psychological well-being and physical activity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 51, 337-347.

Posted in Uncategorized | Leave a comment

Experiential Avoidance: Measurement and Implications for Exercise

About half of American adults don’t meet recommended exercise guidelines and a quarter of American adults don’t exercise at all. Although the various benefits of exercise are well known, why do individuals avoid exercise altogether?

Our research group has begun examining experiential avoidance as one of the reasons why individuals avoid exercise. Experiential avoidance is the tendency to avoid thoughts, feelings, or sensations, even when this avoidance leads to negative outcomes. For example, the physical experiences associated with exercise, like increased heart rate and sweating, can be particularly uncomfortable for some individuals, and are thus experiences to be avoided. However, avoiding exercise also leads to unhealthy outcomes, such as difficulty maintaining a healthy weight, limited physical mobility, and increased disease risk. To understand any psychological phenomenon, it is critical to have an accurate measure of it.

One of the challenges in conducting experiential avoidance research is that measuring experiential avoidance has been difficult. Our research group has recently published a study in the journal Behavior Therapy highlighting the measurement issues associated with experiential avoidance measures. In this large-scale study, we used a series of statistical analyses and found consistent evidence that the two most popular self-report measures of experiential avoidance, the Action and Acceptance Questionnaire-II (AAQ-II) and the Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire (MEAQ), were measuring different things. The AAQ-II was unintentionally measuring the personality trait neuroticism—a proneness to experience negative thoughts and feelings— whereas the MEAQ was measuring it’s intended construct, experiential avoidance. Therefore, the MEAQ is an accurate measure of experiential avoidance.

After determining that the MEAQ is an accurate measure of experiential avoidance, we used it to study why individuals avoid exercise. Because the extent to which an individual enjoys exercise is a marker of how pleasant the experience is for them, we tested the idea that individuals who are high in experiential avoidance who also report low exercise enjoyment would be most likely to avoid exercise. Our results supported this idea. These findings suggest reducing individual’s experiential avoidance could be beneficial for increasing exercise rates.

Taken together, improved measurement of experiential avoidance has allowed our research group to better understand reasons why individuals do not engage in healthy behaviors. This will allow more effective interventions to be developed to target the specific characteristics of an individual to help that individual successfully change their behavior. Future directions include examining experiential avoidance in more health contexts with the goal of incorporating experiential avoidance reduction techniques into health behavior interventions.

Reference: Rochefort, C., Baldwin, A.S., & Chmielewski, M. (2017). Experiential avoidance: An examination of the construct validity of the AAQ-II and MEAQ. Behavior Therapy.

Posted in Uncategorized | Leave a comment

Can Self-Affirmation and Optimism Improve the Health of Cancer Survivors?

With improvements in cancer treatment, Survivor-wristband-ALT-IMAGE-PINK-TEAL-1200_1200more and more individuals will be cancer
survivors. Many of these survivors will face unique physical and mental health challenges after cancer treatment. A recent study published in Annals of Behavioral Medicine suggests that optimism and self-affirmation – the tendency to actively think about the positive aspects of oneself – may promote health in cancer survivors.

Spontaneous self-affirmation refers to actively thinking about the positive traits of oneself when faced with difficult situations, such as having cancer. Optimism is a related personality construct that is characterized by the tendency to view the future positively. In this study, researchers looked at the relation between optimism, spontaneous self-affirmation, and various physical and mental health outcomes, such as cognitive decline, current affect, and information seeking. They used a national sample of cancer survivors from the Health Information National Trends Survey (HINTS). Analysis of large, national datasets allows for more general conclusions to be drawn about cancer survivors.

The main result of this study was that survivors who spontaneously self-affirmed showed greater hopefulness and had a greater likelihood of seeking cancer information. Of note, these relations were still present when controlling for optimism, which suggests that the effect is not simply due to people who self-affirm being more optimistic. Additionally, self-affirming women with a breast cancer diagnosis showed greater confidence in seeking health information, while this was not shown for women with non-breast cancer or men with cancer.

The take home message from this study is that self-affirmation may be beneficial for the mental and physical health of cancer survivors. Although optimism is considered stable across time, self-affirmation may be more amenable to change, and thus can be targeted in interventions to improve the overall health of cancer survivors. Future directions include determining who is likely to self-affirm after a cancer diagnosis and determining whether self-affirmation could lead to increased self-efficacy for managing health.

Reference: Taber, J. M., Klein, W. M., Ferrer, R. A., Kent, E. E., & Harris, P. R. (2015). Optimism and Spontaneous Self-affirmation are Associated with Lower Likelihood of Cognitive Impairment and Greater Positive Affect among Cancer Survivors. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 50(2), 198-209.

Posted in Uncategorized | 1 Comment

Turning Intentions Into Behavior

How many times have you made plans to exercise or eat healthier, but not followed through? You may think “I’ll go to the gym right after work,” but when you get to the end of the day you go home instead. Often we struggle moving from forming intentions to act to actually performing behaviors. An article published this month in Health Psychology suggests that it is easier to turn intentions into behaviors when your intentions are based on how you expect to feel about the behavior.

In this study, researchers examined the relationship between intentions to act and various health behaviors (e.g. eating five servings of fruits and vegetables per day, exercising regularly). Researchers also considered other motives people have that might change this relationship such as how one feels about performing a behavior, or what one believes other think about their behavior. Participants in the UK completed questionnaires at three different times, spanning two months, that assessed their intentions, behaviors, and motives to change that might impact the relation between intentions and behaviors.  The researchers found that when people’s intentions to act are based on expecting to feel regret for not performing a behavior, people were more likely to follow through on their intentions. Despite looking at many possible motives, only the expected feelings of regret mattered in strengthening the relation between intentions and behavior.

The study had several strengths that make the findings particularly compelling.  First, several different health behaviors were examined, so the benefit of basing intentions in anticipated regret is not limited to one single behavior. Instead, the findings suggest anticipating regret strengthens the likelihood of acting for various behaviors. A second strength of the study was that the researchers looked at many potential motives for people’s intentions. Examining various motives strengthens the confidence one has in the finding that anticipated regret is particularly influential in the relation between intentions and behavior. Finally, the researchers were able to examine how anticipated emotions influence the relation between intentions formed at one time and behavior performed later (a prospective study). This aspect of the study design increases confidence that the researchers have identified a meaningful relationship.

These findings suggest that in order to ensure we follow through on our intentions, it may be helpful to base our intentions on how we expect to feel after the behavior rather than focusing on what we think we should do, or how we feel about the behavior. For example, it may be useful to consider the regret we may feel if we do not eat our daily servings of fruits and vegetables or do not exercise.

Reference. Conner, M.C., McEachan, R., Lawton, R., & Gardner, P. (2016). Basis of intentions as a moderator of the intention-health behavior relationship. Health Psychology, 35, 3, 219-227.

Posted in Uncategorized | Leave a comment

Does Receiving Genetic Information About Obesity Change Diet and Exercise Intentions?

Imagine a physician told you that you had a genetic predisposition for obesity. Would you
improve your diet and exercise in order to overcome your predisposition, or would you
decide to not even bother to change your behavior? An article published this year in Annals of Behavioral Medicine suggests that the answer to that question depends on your emotional state. Emotions influence how we interpret and react to information. In medical situations relating to chronic illness, emotions can run high.

In this experiment, researchers looked at the effects of negative emotions on beliefs about the causes of obesity and intentions to change diet and exercise after receiving messages delivered by an online, virtual physician. Participants included overweight women, who were randomized to one of six conditions that varied emotional states and information about the origins of obesity. Randomization is a method used to maximize similarity between groups and helps to rule-out existing differences in participants as an explanation for results.

Negative emotions were induced by asking participants to write about either a fear- or anger-inducing situation they experienced. This method of emotion induction is effective because the emotional situations are personally relevant to the participant. Those in the control condition wrote about a room in their house. Afterwards, participants watched videos of a virtual physician describing the origins of obesity as either genetic or behavioral. Finally, participants completed measures of their beliefs about the causes of obesity and their intentions to improve diet and exercise.

The women who heard the message about genetic predispositions for obesity reported lower intentions to change their diet and exercise, but only when they had written about a fear-inducing experience. For women who wrote about other experiences, the genetic message had no effect on their intentions to change.

The take away message from this study is that the combination of receiving genetic information from a physician while in a fearful state may uniquely lead to reduction of healthy diet and exercise behaviors. Fear can be a common emotion patients experience while interacting with physicians, and this fearful state can lead to unintended behavioral consequences after receiving medical information. Future directions include the need to determine how physicians can attend to patient emotions during encounters to improve behavioral outcomes.

Reference: Persky, S., Ferrer, R. A., & Klein, W. M. (2016). Genomic Information may Inhibit Weight-Related Behavior Change Inclinations Among Individuals in a Fear State. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 1-8.

Posted in Uncategorized | Leave a comment